Struggling to recall a word or name on the
tip of your tongue might not be the sign of a bad memory – and there is an easy
way to prevent it.
Have you ever had
trouble thinking of someone’s name? Perhaps you can even see the face of the
person in your mind’s eye, and you would immediately recognise the name if a
friend suggested it to you. Although this happens frequently with names, it’s
the same for any word. It’s not that you can’t remember the concept but
that you can’t find the language label for it.
Word-finding problems
are an almost stereotypical aspect of the cognitive issues that plague
middle-aged and older adults. These failures occur without warning for even the
most familiar words and names a person knows. The most troublesome words,
researchers have found, are proper
nouns and the names of objects. This retrieval inability can last anywhere from
a split second to minutes or even hours, and they can be exasperating. In fact,
older adults frequently mention word-finding problems when asked about the
annoyances of ageing.
In such cases, a
person is certain she knows the word she is searching for. It may seem as if
the awol term is just on the tip of her tongue, but for some reason she can’t
produce it, at least at that moment. In fact, psychologists refer to such
experiences as tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states. But are they really the
harbingers of befuddlement that they appear to be?
Alamy
Certain
word games can trigger the frustrating tip of the tongue phenomenon (Credit:
Alamy)
Advertisement
Studying TOT presents
certain challenges to psychologists who want to understand how and why such
states occur. Much like astronomers who study ephemeral phenomena like
supernovas, researchers know that TOT states will eventually happen, but not
exactly when. This uncertainty has led to two distinctly different ways of
investigating TOTs: via naturalistic methods and by experimentally inducing
word-finding failures in laboratory settings.
Researchers studying
word finding and TOT have tried to quantify two aspects in particular: how
often these states occur and the likelihood that they are resolved – that is,
the sought-after word is spontaneously recalled by the person without external
assistance (such as looking the word up or having a friend offer the solution).
Diary studies, in
which people write down every time they experience a TOT state, allow
researchers to assess both frequency and resolution rates. The results suggest
that college students experience about one to two TOT states a week, while for
people in their 60s and early 70s, the rate is slightly higher. Research
participants in their 80s, however, experience TOT states at a rate almost twice as
high as college students. Diary studies have shown that TOT episodes are likely
to be resolved – the typical success rate in such studies is more than 90%.
Simply
giving participants dictionary definitions of uncommon English words would
often trigger a word-finding failure
We need to be
cautious, however, when interpreting such naturalistic data. It may be the case
that older adults, who are more concerned about their memory lapses, will be
more likely to record such instances. They may be more conscientious about
writing them down, perhaps because their lives are less hectic than those of
younger participants. It may also be the case that participants are simply more
likely to record resolved TOT states than episodes that are not resolved.
Advertisement
The alternative
method for studying word finding is to experimentally induce a TOT state. A
method for doing this was developed by psychologists Roger Brown and David
McNeill when they were both at Harvard University. They found that simply
giving participants dictionary definitions of uncommon English words would
often trigger a word-finding failure. An example from their study was: “A
navigational instrument used in measuring angular distances, especially the altitude
of the Sun, Moon, and stars at sea.”
(If this example has
caused a TOT state for you, then the word you are searching for is “sextant”.)
In this study, the
participants were often able to provide the desired word without difficulty. On
other occasions, the subjects had no idea what word the definition was
describing. However, if they found themselves in a TOT state, Brown and McNeill
asked them additional questions. The researchers discovered that, while in such
a state, people can report partial information about the sought-after word,
even as the word itself eludes their grasp.
Solvin
Zankl/Alamy
It is a
sextant, not a sextet or a sexton, in case you are struggling to find the right
word (Credit: Solvin Zankl/Alamy)
For example, the
participants performed far above chance when asked to guess how many syllables
the word had, or what its initial letter might be. And not surprisingly, when
people made errors, they often produced words that had a similar meaning. When
given the definition for “sextant”, the participants sometimes responded with
“astrolabe” or “compass”. However, they also sometimes offered up words that
only sounded like the intended term. The definition for “sextant” also led to
responses of “sextet” and “sexton”.
Advertisement
You might also like:
·
The people with no word for
dinosaur
·
The surprising power of reading
aloud
·
The words that make you human
If we assume that
sailors wielding their sextants are neither members of six-person musical
groups nor gravediggers, then these errors suggest something important about
how our knowledge of words is arranged in memory. Studies with older adults, however, suggest that
partial information – such as the initial letter of the word – is less
available for them.
As with many issues
in cognitive aging, we can view the increase in TOT states as a glass half
empty or half full. On the one hand, these retrieval failures can be taken as evidence of
weakening connections between the meanings of concepts and the words that
denote them in long-term memory. It’s also possible that the increase in
word-finding problems with age reflects something very different.
Psychologist Donna
Dahlgren at Indiana University Southeast has argued that the key issue is not one of age but
one of knowledge. If older adults typically have more information in long-term
memory, then as a consequence they will experience more TOT states.
Advertisement
Research
suggests you might have fewer such episodes if you maintain your aerobic
fitness
It’s also possible
that TOT states are useful – they can serve as a signal to the older adult that
the sought-for word is known, even if not currently accessible. Such
metacognitive information is beneficial because it signals that spending more
time trying to resolve the word-finding failure may ultimately lead to success.
Viewed this way, TOT
states might represent not retrieval failures but valuable sources of
information. If you are an older adult and still worried about the number of
TOT states that you experience, research suggests you might have fewer such
episodes if you maintain your aerobic fitness.
So, the next time you
have trouble thinking of a word, you can also try looking for it around the
block.
* This article originally
appeared in The
MIT Press Reader, and is republished with permission. Roger Kreuz is
a professor of psychology at the University of Memphis. Richard Roberts is
a foreign service officer currently serving at the US Consulate General in
Okinawa, Japan. They are the co-authors of “Changing Minds: How Aging
Affects Language and How Language Affects Aging”, from which
this article is excerpted.
Comments
Post a Comment